Like what you've read?

On Line Opinion is the only Australian site where you get all sides of the story. We don't
charge, but we need your support. Here�s how you can help.

  • Advertise

    We have a monthly audience of 70,000 and advertising packages from $200 a month.

  • Volunteer

    We always need commissioning editors and sub-editors.

  • Contribute

    Got something to say? Submit an essay.


 The National Forum   Donate   Your Account   On Line Opinion   Forum   Blogs   Polling   About   
On Line Opinion logo ON LINE OPINION - Australia's e-journal of social and political debate

Subscribe!
Subscribe





On Line Opinion is a not-for-profit publication and relies on the generosity of its sponsors, editors and contributors. If you would like to help, contact us.
___________

Syndicate
RSS/XML


RSS 2.0

Improving population health means more than reducing inequality

By Richard Eckersley - posted Friday, 11 December 2015


Growing concern over rising inequality has renewed interest in the effects of inequality on health. But the science is not as straightforward as many think.

There was a time, about 20 years ago, when research into inequality and health seemed to tell a simple, coherent and compelling story. The research showed that there were social gradients in health, such that at any point in the social hierarchy, people on average had worse health than those above them and better health than those below them. More unequal societies had more unequal health--i.e., steeper gradients in health. And more unequal societies appeared to have poorer average health--that is, inequality was bad for everyone's health, not just those of lower socio-economic status.

However, as public and political interest in inequality and health has increased, the scientific story appears to have become less clear. It remains true that poverty and disadvantage harm health, and that most societies have social gradients in health. But more unequal societies do not necessarily have more unequal health, raising doubts about whether reducing inequality would reduce health inequalities. And whether more unequal societies have worse health overall remains contested and inconclusive, despite hundreds of studies over several decades. Empirical findings are inconsistent and contradictory and there is still no consensus among researchers.

Advertisement

Even by the early 2000s, some reviews were challenging the view that income inequality was a major determinant of differences in population health. The debate continues to this day. A 2009 study found only a 'modest' effect of income inequality on health, and called for further investigations. A 2014 study of 28 European countries showed that there were simple correlations between income inequality and a range of measures of health and wellbeing. However, unequal societies were on average much poorer; once per capita GDP was taken into account, national inequality did not reduce health or wellbeing. 'These results all imply that directing policies and resources towards inequality reduction is unlikely to benefit the general public in advanced societies', the authors say.

In the public debate, the 2009 best-selling book, The spirit level: why equality is better for everyone, by British epidemiologists Richard Wilkinson and Kate Pickett has attracted most attention. Reflecting the emphasis on inequality in the research on social determinants in general, Wilkinson and Pickett attach great importance to tackling inequality in shaping social outcomes, including improving the health and quality of life of all of us.

More than this, reducing inequality would help us to address other problems: we can regain a sense of optimism that social and environmental problems can be solved, they write, knowing that 'greater equality will help us rein in consumerism and ease the introduction of policies to tackle global warming'. Their vision has seen health inequalities become part of broader debates about progress and sustainability.

At the core of Wilkinson and Pickett's argument is the effect of inequality on social relations, which in turn affect health: 'greater equality is the material foundation on which better social relations are built', they write. In explaining how inequality gets 'under the skin', they stress the importance of social judgements and evaluations to wellbeing, and of inequality as a cause of 'social evaluation anxieties'.

At the most fundamental level, they write, reducing inequality is about 'shifting the balance from the divisive, self-interested consumerism driven by status competition, towards a more socially integrated and affiliative society'. 'Greater equality can help us develop the public ethos and commitment to working together which we need if we are going to solve the problems which threaten us all.'

However, it is conceptually and empirically flawed to attribute qualities like self-interest and consumerism so narrowly to inequality. It is not obvious, for example, that income inequality would have more impact on social relations, including social comparisons and status competition, than the way modern Western culture construes the self and its relations to others through qualities such as individualism and materialism.

Advertisement

If perceptions of social status influence levels of stress and anxiety, then cultural factors also play a critical role: for example, amplifying a sense of relative deprivation through media images of 'the good life' and celebrity lifestyles that are increasingly beyond the reach of most people; or moderating it by providing alternative cultural models, such as downshifting and simple living, that undermine conventional social comparisons. Culture also influences the social distribution of risky behaviour like smoking and alcohol use, modifying income-related health gradients and helping to explain why more unequal countries do not always have more unequal health.

More importantly, culture has a wider significance as a system of meanings and symbols that defines how people see the world and their place in it, and gives meaning to personal and collective experience. Every aspect of reality is embedded within webs of meaning that define a certain worldview and that vary with individuals, times and societies. In other words, cultures define what we know about the world, and so what we do; in this sense they are fundamental to population health and wellbeing.

Research shows, for example, that materialism--giving priority to money and what it buys--has increased among young Americans over several generations, a rise attributed to cultural reinforcement and social instability and disconnection). Materialism was already rising rapidly in the 1970s (the earliest data used in the study) and peaked in about 1990, so the trend does not track that of inequality. Materialistic values are associated with: lower life satisfaction, happiness and vitality, and higher depression and anxiety; less prosocial and cooperative behaviours, and more antisocial and competitive behaviours; and more environmentally damaging and unsustainable choices and lifestyles.

  1. Pages:
  2. Page 1
  3. 2
  4. All

This article is adapted from a paper, 'Beyond inequality: Acknowledging the complexity of social determinants of health', published recently in the journal Social Science & Medicine. The paper is also available on www.richardeckersley.com.au.



Discuss in our Forums

See what other readers are saying about this article!

Click here to read & post comments.

1 post so far.

Share this:
reddit this reddit thisbookmark with del.icio.us Del.icio.usdigg thisseed newsvineSeed NewsvineStumbleUpon StumbleUponsubmit to propellerkwoff it

About the Author

Richard Eckersley is an independent researcher. His work explores progress and wellbeing.

Other articles by this Author

All articles by Richard Eckersley

Creative Commons LicenseThis work is licensed under a Creative Commons License.

Photo of Richard Eckersley
Article Tools
Comment 1 comment
Print Printable version
Subscribe Subscribe
Email Email a friend
Advertisement

About Us Search Discuss Feedback Legals Privacy